Stuttering Research - General
Research
March
2010
The five factor
model of personality applied to adults who stutter.
Iverach L, O'Brian S, Jones M,
Block S, Lincoln M, Harrison E, Hewat S, Menzies RG, Packman A,
Onslow M.
Australian Stuttering Research Centre, The
University of Sydney, PO Box 170, Lidcombe, NSW 1825,
Australia.
Abstract
Previous research has not explored the Five Factor Model
of personality among adults who stutter. Therefore, the
present study investigated the five personality domains of
Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness and
Conscientiousness, as measured by the NEO Five Factor
Inventory (NEO-FFI), in a sample of 93 adults seeking
speech treatment for stuttering, and compared these scores
with normative data from an Australian and a United States
sample. Results revealed that NEO-FFI scores for the
stuttering group were within the 'average' range for all
five personality domains. However, adults who stutter were
characterized by significantly higher Neuroticism, and
significantly lower Agreeableness and Conscientiousness,
than normative samples. No significant differences were
found between groups on the dimensions of Extraversion and
Openness. These results are discussed with reference to the
relationship between personality factors among adults who
stutter, their directionality, and implications for
predicting treatment outcome. LEARNING OUTCOMES: The reader
will be able to: (1) describe the Five Factor Model of
personality, including the NEO-FFI personality domains of
Extraversion, Neuroticism, Openness, Agreeableness, and
Conscientiousness, and (2) discuss differences in NEO-FFI
domain scores between adults who stutter and normative
samples, and (3) understand the clinical implications of
personality profiles in terms of treatment process and
outcome for adults who stutter.
January
2010
Social
anxiety disorder in adults who stutter.
Blumgart E, Tran Y, Craig
A.
Rehabilitation Studies Unit, Northern
Clinical School, Faculty of Medicine, The University of
Sydney, Ryde, New South Wales, Australia.
Abstract
Background: The nature and prevalence of social
anxiety disorder (social phobia (SP)) in people who
stutter is uncertain, and DSM-IV differential diagnosis
guidelines make it difficult to classify an adult who
stutters (AWS) with SP as it is assumed any social
anxiety symptoms will be a consequence of their
stuttering. The aim of this study was to determine the
spot prevalence of SP in AWS and to investigate
differences in social anxiety between AWS and controls
who do not stutter. Methods: The study involved a
comprehensive assessment of 200 AWS and 200 adults who
do not stutter similar in age and sex ratio. Measures
included stuttering severity, health status,
self-report measures of social anxiety as well as a
structured diagnostic interview for SP for randomly
selected sub-group of 50 from each group. Results: The
AWS were found to have significantly raised trait and
social anxiety, as well as significantly increased risk
of SP in comparison to the controls. Findings indicated
a SP spot prevalence of at least 40% in AWS, and for
them to be at high risk of having Generalized SP.
Conclusions: It is concluded that the DSM-IV diagnostic
guidelines for diagnosing SP in AWS could result in
professional confusion and have possible negative
mental health ramifications. Implications for the
psychological and medical treatment of AWS are
discussed. Depression and Anxiety 0:1-6, 2010. (c) 2010
Wiley-Liss, Inc.
May
2009
The effects of self-generated
synchronous and asynchronous visual speech feedback on overt
stuttering frequency.
Snyder GJ, Hough MS, Blanchet P,
Ivy LJ, Waddell D.
The Laboratory for Stuttering Research, Department of
Communication Sciences & Disorders, University of
Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA.
GSnyder@OleMiss.edu
PURPOSE: Relatively recent research
documents that visual choral speech, which represents an
externally generated form of synchronous visual speech
feedback, significantly enhanced fluency in those who stutter.
As a consequence, it was hypothesized that self-generated
synchronous and asynchronous visual speech feedback would
likewise enhance fluency. Therefore, the purpose of this study
was to investigate the effects of self-generated visual
feedback (i.e., synchronous speech feedback with a mirror and
asynchronous speech feedback via delayed visual feedback) on
overt stuttering frequency in those who stutter. METHOD: Eight
people who stutter (4 males, 4 females), ranging from 18 to 42
years of age participated in this study. Due to the nature of
visual speech feedback, the speaking task required that
participants recite memorized phrases in control and
experimental speaking conditions so that visual attention could
be focused on the speech feedback, rather than a written
passage. During experimental conditions, participants recited
memorized phrases while simultaneously focusing on the movement
of their lips, mouth, and jaw within their own synchronous
(i.e., mirror) and asynchronous (i.e., delayed video signal)
visual speech feedback. RESULTS: Results indicated that the
self-generated visual feedback speaking conditions
significantly decreased stuttering frequency
(Greenhouse-Geisser p=.000); post hoc orthogonal comparisons
revealed no significant differences in stuttering frequency
reduction between the synchronous and asynchronous visual
feedback speaking conditions (p=.2554). CONCLUSIONS: These data
suggest that synchronous and asynchronous self-generated visual
speech feedback is associated with significant reductions in
overt stuttering frequency. Study results were discussed
relative to existing theoretical models of fluency-enhancement
via speech feedback, such as the engagement of mirror neuron
networks, the EXPLAN model, and the Dual Premotor System
Hypothesis. Further research in the area of self-generated
visual speech feedback, as well as theoretical constructs
accounting for how exposure to a multi-sensory speech feedback
enhances fluency, is warranted. LEARNING OUTCOMES: : Readers
will be able to (1) discuss the multi-sensory nature of
fluency-enhancing speech feedback, (2) compare and contrast
synchronous and asynchronous self-generated and externally
generated visual speech feedback, and (3) compare and contrast
self-generated and externally generated visual speech
feedback.
April
2009
Influence of stuttering
variation on talker group classification in preschool children:
preliminary
findings.
Johnson KN, Karrass J, Conture EG,
Walden T.
Department of Communication Sciences
& Disorders, Stuttering Research Laboratory, James Madison
University, Harrisonburg, VA 22801, USA. johns3kn@jmu.edu
The purpose of this study was to
investigate whether variations in disfluencies of young
children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) significantly
change their talker group classification or diagnosis from
stutterer to nonstutterer, and vice versa. Participants
consisted of seventeen 3- to 5-year-old CWS and nine 3- to
5-year-old CWNS, with no statistically significant
between-group difference in chronological age (CWS: M=45.53
months, S.D.=8.32; CWNS: M=47.67 months, S.D.=6.69). All
participants had speech, language, and hearing development
within normal limits, with the exception of stuttering for CWS.
Both talker groups participated in a series of speaking samples
that varied by: (a) conversational partner [parent and
clinician], (b) location [home and clinic], and (c) context
[conversation and narrative]. The primary dependent measures
for this study were the number of stuttering-like disfluencies
(SLD) per total number of spoken words [%SLD] and the ratio of
SLD to total disfluencies (TD) [SLD/TD]. The results indicated
that significant variability of stuttering did not exist as a
result of conversational partner or location. Changes in
context, however, did impact the CWS, who demonstrated higher
SLD/TD in the conversation sample versus a narrative sample.
Consistent with hypotheses, CWS and CWNS were accurately
identified as stutterers and nonstutterers, respectively,
regardless of changes to conversational partner, location or
context for the overall participant sample. Present findings
were taken to suggest that during assessment, variations in
stuttering frequency resulting from changes in conversational
partner, location or context do not significantly influence the
diagnosis of stuttering, especially for children not on the
talker group classification borderline between CWS and CWNS.
LEARNING OUTCOMES: Readers will be able to: (1) Describe the
role of variability in stuttering frequency for young children
who stutter; (2) Identify three fundamental measurements of the
frequency of stuttering-like and nonstuttering-like
disfluencies; (3) Describe the effects of stuttering variation
on talker group classification of stuttering or
nonstuttering.
Influence of stuttering
variation on talker group classification in preschool children:
preliminary
findings.
Johnson KN, Karrass
J, Conture
EG, Walden
T.
Department of Communication Sciences
& Disorders, Stuttering Research Laboratory, James Madison
University, Harrisonburg, VA 22801, USA. johns3kn@jmu.edu
The purpose of this study was to
investigate whether variations in disfluencies of young
children who do (CWS) and do not stutter (CWNS) significantly
change their talker group classification or diagnosis from
stutterer to nonstutterer, and vice versa. Participants
consisted of seventeen 3- to 5-year-old CWS and nine 3- to
5-year-old CWNS, with no statistically significant
between-group difference in chronological age (CWS: M=45.53
months, S.D.=8.32; CWNS: M=47.67 months, S.D.=6.69). All
participants had speech, language, and hearing development
within normal limits, with the exception of stuttering for CWS.
Both talker groups participated in a series of speaking samples
that varied by: (a) conversational partner [parent and
clinician], (b) location [home and clinic], and (c) context
[conversation and narrative]. The primary dependent measures
for this study were the number of stuttering-like disfluencies
(SLD) per total number of spoken words [%SLD] and the ratio of
SLD to total disfluencies (TD) [SLD/TD]. The results indicated
that significant variability of stuttering did not exist as a
result of conversational partner or location. Changes in
context, however, did impact the CWS, who demonstrated higher
SLD/TD in the conversation sample versus a narrative sample.
Consistent with hypotheses, CWS and CWNS were accurately
identified as stutterers and nonstutterers, respectively,
regardless of changes to conversational partner, location or
context for the overall participant sample. Present findings
were taken to suggest that during assessment, variations in
stuttering frequency resulting from changes in conversational
partner, location or context do not significantly influence the
diagnosis of stuttering, especially for children not on the
talker group classification borderline between CWS and CWNS.
LEARNING OUTCOMES: Readers will be able to: (1) Describe the
role of variability in stuttering frequency for young children
who stutter; (2) Identify three fundamental measurements of the
frequency of stuttering-like and nonstuttering-like
disfluencies; (3) Describe the effects of stuttering variation
on talker group classification of stuttering or
nonstuttering.
Characteristics of stuttering
in Dutch-speaking individuals.
Boey RA, Wuyts FL, van de Heyning PH,
Heylen L, de Bodt MS.
Centre of Stuttering Therapy
Antwerp.
Clinical diagnostic procedures to
distinguish stuttering from non-stuttering individuals
partially rely on the observation of characteristics of
stuttered words or syllables. The purpose of this study was to
examine the sensitivity and specificity of such a procedure,
and to describe the observed characteristics of stuttered
words. Methods involved the recording of the frequency,
duration, and physical tension of stuttered words in
conversational speech samples (total 43 100 words) and in an
oral reading task (total 43 100 words). In this manner, 351
native Dutch speaking people, aged 8-53, who stutter were
compared to a group of non-stuttering peers (n = 80). Findings
suggest that applying a criterion of 3% stuttered words in
conversational speech makes it possible to distinguish
stuttering from non-stuttering individuals with high
sensitivity (.9345) and perfect specificity (1.0000).
Frequency, duration, and physical tension accompanying
stuttered words are described for the group of stuttering
participants, and these characteristics were compared to the
group of non-stuttering speakers.
Does language influence the
accuracy of judgments of stuttering in children?
Einarsdóttir J, Ingham RJ.
University of Iceland.
PURPOSE: To determine whether stuttering
judgment accuracy is influenced by familiarity with the
stuttering speaker's language. METHOD: Audiovisual 7-min speech
samples from nine 3-5 y.o. Icelandic children who stutter
(CWS), pre-selected for different levels of stuttering, were
subdivided into 5-s intervals. Ten experienced Icelandic
Speech-Language Pathologists (ICE-SLPs) and 10 experienced US
Speech-Language Pathologists (US-SLPs), the latter being
unfamiliar with the Icelandic language, independently judged
each 5-s interval (n = 756) as stuttered or nonstuttered on two
separate occasions. RESULTS: As in previous studies, intervals
judged to contain stuttering showed wide variability within the
ICE-SLP and US-SLP groups. However, both SLP groups (1)
displayed satisfactory mean intrajudge agreement, (2) met an
independent stuttering judgment accuracy criterion test using
English-speaking CWS samples, and (3) met an agreement
criterion on approximately 90% of their stuttering and
nonstuttering judgments on the Icelandic-speaking CWS samples.
CONCLUSIONS: Experienced SLPs were shown to be highly accurate
in recognizing stuttering and nonstuttering exemplars from
young CWS speaking in an unfamiliar language. The findings
suggest that judgments of occurrences of stuttering in CWS are
not generally language dependent, although some exceptions were
noted.
Speech rate according to
stuttering severity.
Arcuri CF, Osborn E, Schiefer AM,
Chiari BM.
Departamento de Fonoaudiologia, Escola
Paulista de Medicina, Universidade Federal de São Paulo.
clauarcuri@gmail.com
BACKGROUND: Speech duration has been the
subject of acoustic studies due to its relationship with rhythm
and speech rate. The speech analysis of stutterers has revealed
data which often differs from that found in non-stutterers.
These differences most likely stem from timing disturbances
related to speech motor control. OBJECTIVE: To compare the
speech rates of individuals presenting different stuttering
severity levels. METHOD: Participants were 6 adult stutterers
with the following severity levels: 2 with mild stuttering; 2
with moderate stuttering and 2 with severe stuttering. The
words 'cavalo' (horse), 'pipoca' (popcorn) and 'tapete'
(carpet) were introduced in a carrier phrase,
'Digo......baixinho' (Say ......in a low voice). Each phrase
was uttered by the researcher and then repeated aloud 3 times
by the participants while being recorded in a computer.
Utterances containing speech disruptions were discarded.
Subsequently, timing measurements were made, using the Praat
4.2 software. Utterances were divided into segments delimited
by two consecutive voice onsets, and speech rate was then
calculated (number of vowel-vowel segments divided by the total
sum of duration of the segments). The data were analyzed
statistically using ANOVA. RESULTS: The group with
mild/moderate stuttering presented higher and similar speech
rates, differing statistically from the group with severe
stuttering, therefore indicating that the higher the severity
of stuttering, the lower the speech rate. This difference seems
to be related to difficulties in motor programming, affecting
mainly the rhythm and the timing of discourse. CONCLUSIONS: The
rate of fluent speech during a repetition task differentiated
the studied individuals according to the severity of
stuttering.
A life-time of stuttering:
How emotional reactions to stuttering impact activities and
participation in older people.
Bricker-Katz G, Lincoln M, McCabe P.
Australian Stuttering Research Centre,
The University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia.
Purpose: The International Classification
of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF) framework has a
pragmatic focus on how impairment impacts the individual's
activities and participation. Stuttering is known to impact
communication in younger adults but this has not been
established in older people who stutter. In this study,
emotional reactions to stuttering were investigated in a group
55 years and older who self-reported stuttering since
childhood. Method: This was a cross-sectional descriptive
design. Twelve participants who self-reported that they still
stuttered and in whom stuttering was confirmed, and 14 controls
completed the Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale (FNES), The
Endler Multi-dimensional Anxiety Scales-Trait (EMAS-T) and The
Australian Personal Wellbeing Index (PWA-I). Participants whose
stuttering persisted also completed the Overall Assessment of
Speakers Experience of Stuttering (OASES). Results: The group
who stuttered scored significantly higher on the FNES, with
scores in the social phobia range. Responses on the OASES
showed that stuttering continues to be a negative experience
for this older group. Results for the EMAS-T and PWA-I were
within the average range across both participant groups however
significant differences existed between the groups in the
social evaluative and physical danger domains of the EMAS-T,
and the satisfaction with health domain of the PWA-I.
Conclusions: Significant fear of negative evaluation, which is
the key feature for social anxiety, was found in the group of
older people who stuttered with a higher level of trait anxiety
in social evaluative domains. The OASES showed that they also
reacted to stuttering and communication in daily situations
with moderate to severe impact scores which showed that
stuttering impacted on speaking activities and by those
negative experiences limited communication. Limited
communication and restricted participation in the lives of
older people have implications for healthy productive ageing
and this is discussed.
Peer Responses to Stuttering
in the Preschool Setting.
Langevin M, Packman A, Onslow M.
Australian Stuttering Research Centre,
The University of Sydney, Australia.
PURPOSE: This study investigated peer
responses to preschoolers' stuttering in preschool and sought
to determine if specific characteristics of participants'
stuttering pattern elicited negative peer responses. METHOD:
Four outdoor free play sessions of four 3-4 year old
preschoolers who stutter were videotaped. Stutters were
identified on transcripts of the play sessions. Peer responses
to stuttered utterances were judged to be negative or
neutral/positive. Thereafter, participants' stuttering
behaviors, durations of stutters, and judgments of the
meaningfulness of peer-directed stuttered utterances were
analyzed. RESULTS: Between 71.4% and 100% of peer responses
were judged to be neutral/positive. In the minority of negative
responses across three participants, peers were observed to
react with confusion or to interrupt, mock, walk away from, or
ignore the stuttered utterances. Utterances that elicited
negative responses were typically meaningless and contained
stutters that were behaviorally complex and/or of longer
duration. Other social interaction difficulties also were
observed, for example, difficulty leading peers in play,
participating in pretend play, and resolving conflicts.
CONCLUSIONS: Results indicate that the majority of peer
responses to stuttered utterances were neutral/positive;
however, results also indicate that stuttering has the
potential to elicit negative peer responses and affect other
social interactions in preschool.
December
2008
Social anxiety and the
severity and typography of stuttering in
adolescents.
Mulcahy K, Hennessey N, Beilby J,
Byrnes M.
School of Psychology, Curtin University
of Technology, GPO Box U 1987, Perth, Western Australia, 6845,
Australia.
The present study examined the relationship
between anxiety, attitude toward daily communication, and
stuttering symptomatology in adolescent stuttering. Adolescents
who stuttered (n=19) showed significantly higher levels of
trait, state and social anxiety than fluent speaking controls
(n=18). Trait and state anxiety was significantly associated
with difficulty with communication in daily situations for
adolescents who stutter, but not for controls. No statistically
significant associations were found between anxiety and
measures of communication difficulty, and the severity or
typography of stuttering surface behaviours. These results
highlight some of the psychosocial concomitants of chronic
stuttering in adolescence, but challenge the notion that
anxiety plays a direct mediating role in stuttering surface
behaviours. Rather, the results suggest stuttering is a
disorder that features psychosocial conflict regardless of its
surface features. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be
able to: (1) summarise findings from previous studies with
regards to stuttering and anxiety; (2) identify the sub-types
of anxiety that may impact on the individual who stutters; and
(3) discuss the clinical implications of the results with
regards to working with adolescents who stutter.
Non-linguistic auditory
processing in stuttering: evidence from behavior and
event-related brain potentials.
Hampton A, Weber-Fox C.
Purdue University, Speech, Language,
& Hearing Sciences, Heavilon Hall, 500 Oval Drive, West
Lafayette, IN 47907, USA.
Auditory processing deficits are
hypothesized to play a role in the disorder of stuttering (e.g.
Hall, J. W., & Jerger, J. (1978). Central auditory function
in stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 21,
324-337). The current study focused on non-linguistic auditory
processing without verbal responses to explore the relationship
between behavior and neural activity in the absence of
cognitive demands related to language processing and
articulatory planning for speaking. A pure-tone, oddball
paradigm was utilized to compare behavioral accuracy and
reaction times for adults who stutter (AWS) and normally fluent
speakers (NFS). Additionally, event-related potentials elicited
by brief standard and target tones were compared for the two
groups. Results revealed that, as a group, AWS tended to
perform less accurately compared to the NFS and were slower to
respond to target stimuli. However, inspection of individual
data indicated that most of the AWS performed within the range
of normally fluent speakers while a small subset of AWS were
well outside the normal range. This subgroup of AWS also
demonstrated early perceptual processes (as indexed by N100 and
P200 amplitudes) indicative of reduced cortical representation
of auditory input. The P300 mean amplitudes elicited in AWS
tended to be reduced overall compared to those of the NFS,
suggesting the possibility of weaker updates in working memory
for representations of the target tone stimuli in AWS. Taken
together, these findings point to the possibility that a subset
of AWS exhibit non-linguistic auditory processing deficits
related to altered cortical processing. Educational objectives:
After reading this article, the reader will be able to: (1)
summarize research findings of non-linguistic auditory
processing in stuttering; (2) discuss the relationship between
behavioral performance for auditory processing and the
underlying event-related brain potentials; (3) discuss the
importance of analyses of individual versus group data in
stuttering; and (4) summarize how the findings of this study
relate to a multifactorial model of stuttering.
The effects of self-generated
synchronous and asynchronous visual speech feedback on overt
stuttering frequency.
Snyder GJ, Hough MS, Blanchet P,
Ivy LJ, Waddell D.
The Laboratory for Stuttering Research,
Department of Communication Sciences & Disorders,
University of Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA.
GSnyder@OleMiss.edu
PURPOSE: Relatively recent research
documents that visual choral speech, which represents an
externally generated form of synchronous visual speech
feedback, significantly enhanced fluency in those who stutter.
As a consequence, it was hypothesized that self-generated
synchronous and asynchronous visual speech feedback would
likewise enhance fluency. Therefore, the purpose of this study
was to investigate the effects of self-generated visual
feedback (i.e., synchronous speech feedback with a mirror and
asynchronous speech feedback via delayed visual feedback) on
overt stuttering frequency in those who stutter. METHOD: Eight
people who stutter (4 males, 4 females), ranging from 18 to 42
years of age participated in this study. Due to the nature of
visual speech feedback, the speaking task required that
participants recite memorized phrases in control and
experimental speaking conditions so that visual attention could
be focused on the speech feedback, rather than a written
passage. During experimental conditions, participants recited
memorized phrases while simultaneously focusing on the movement
of their lips, mouth, and jaw within their own synchronous
(i.e., mirror) and asynchronous (i.e., delayed video signal)
visual speech feedback. RESULTS: Results indicated that the
self-generated visual feedback speaking conditions
significantly decreased stuttering frequency
(Greenhouse-Geisser p=.000); post hoc orthogonal comparisons
revealed no significant differences in stuttering frequency
reduction between the synchronous and asynchronous visual
feedback speaking conditions (p=.2554). CONCLUSIONS: These data
suggest that synchronous and asynchronous self-generated visual
speech feedback is associated with significant reductions in
overt stuttering frequency. Study results were discussed
relative to existing theoretical models of fluency-enhancement
via speech feedback, such as the engagement of mirror neuron
networks, the EXPLAN model, and the Dual Premotor System
Hypothesis. Further research in the area of self-generated
visual speech feedback, as well as theoretical constructs
accounting for how exposure to a multi-sensory speech feedback
enhances fluency, is warranted. LEARNING OUTCOMES: : Readers
will be able to (1) discuss the multi-sensory nature of
fluency-enhancing speech feedback, (2) compare and contrast
synchronous and asynchronous self-generated and externally
generated visual speech feedback, and (3) compare and contrast
self-generated and externally generated visual speech
feedback.
Older people who stutter:
barriers to communication and perceptions of treatment
needs.
Bricker-Katz G, Lincoln M, McCabe P.
Australian Stuttering Research Centre,
University of Sydney, Lidcombe, Australia.
Background: Little is known about the
experience of stuttering for people over 55 years of age.
Recent research has established that the same types of
stuttering behaviours, cognitions, and emotional consequences
experienced during young adulthood persist into older age.
Aims: The aims were to investigate perceptions of limitations
to activity and participation in a group of older people who
stuttered into adulthood. A further aim was to find out their
perceptions about treatment. Methods & Procedures: This was
a qualitative study involving eleven participants, eight males
and three females over 55 years of age (mean age = 70.7,
standard deviation = 9.13 years, range = 57.2-83.8 years) who
self-reported stuttering into adulthood. Participants were
randomly assigned to two focus groups for the discussion of
topic questions posed by a moderator. The discussion was video-
and audio-recorded, transcribed, and analysed using a
comparative thematic analysis to derive emergent themes in
relation to the topic questions. Outcomes & Results:
Stuttering can impact on the lives of older people in a similar
way to younger people who stutter. Participants who continued
to work felt more limited by their stuttering because work
involved unpredictable speaking situations with unfamiliar
people. Others who had retired experienced some relief from
these limitations because they were no longer required to
communicate in a work context. The acceptance of stuttering was
a theme expressed by some participants, and acceptance
diminished the limitations because these older people were less
fearful of the consequences of their stuttering. However,
others remained constrained by the impact of stuttering on
their communication and struggled with a fear of speaking and a
fear of negative evaluation by others. They applied learnt and
self-devised techniques to assist their speech and felt that if
fear of speaking was removed and their self-confidence
increased, communication might be better. They would like
effective, individual, and short-term treatment with
speech-language pathologists who are knowledgeable about
stuttering and sensitive to their emotional needs. Conclusions
& Implications: Older people who stutter experienced
limitations to participation because of their stuttering and
there are implications for their future ability to remain
independent and connected to relevant people and services.
Further investigation of these limitations and research into
effective intervention is indicated.
Characteristics of disfluency
clusters in adults who stutter.
Robb MP, Sargent A, O'Beirne GA.
Department of Communication Disorders,
University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.
michael.robb@canterbury.ac.nz
BACKGROUND/AIMS: The purpose of this study
was to examine characteristics of disfluency clusters in adults
who stutter (AWS) and to compare these characteristics to those
previously reported for children who stutter (CWS). METHOD: The
spontaneous speech of ten AWS was sampled and organized
according to utterance length in syllables. The overall number
and type of disfluency clusters occurring in each sample were
determined. RESULTS: Findings indicated that utterances
containing disfluency clusters were significantly longer than
fluent utterances, and the occurrence of disfluency clusters
was correlated with overall percentage of disfluency.
CONCLUSION: The results obtained in the present study for AWS
tend to parallel those found for CWS and serve to validate
their occurrence as a feature of the disorder of
stuttering.
Students' perceptions of
face-to-face pseudostuttering experience.
Lohman P.
California State University, East Bay,
Communicative Sciences and Disorders Department, 25800 Carlos
Bee Blvd., MB 1099, Hayward, CA 94542-3035, USA.
patricia.lohmanhawk@csueastbay.edu
Students' perceptions of simulating
stuttering in public were obtained in order to quantify
students' increased understanding of stuttering. 34 women and 2
men (M age=30.0 yr., SD=9.0) majoring in communication
disorders participated. All were graduate students enrolled in
their first class in fluency disorders. None had previous
experience with individuals who stuttered. Participants
completed a questionnaire before and after the experiment and
discussed these experiences with the group. Responses to 39
questions using a 5-pt. Likert-type scale were analyzed for
students' preparation, feelings, attitudes, and perception of
listeners' reactions. Participants recounted both positive and
negative experiences gaining understanding of and empathy for
people who stutter, changing erroneous perceptions and
broadening perspective of the disorder. Participants reported
that the roundtable discussion contributed to their learning.
Assigning students to simulate stuttering in public and
participate in a follow-up discussion is an effective
evidence-based teaching practice.
Stuttering in
english-mandarin bilingual speakers: the influence of language
dominance on stuttering severity.
Lim VP, Lincoln M, Chan YH, Onslow
M.
Speech Therapy Department, Singapore
General Hospital, Outram Road, Singapore 169608. E-mail:
valerie.lim.p.c@sgh.com.sg.
PURPOSE: English and Mandarin are the 2
most spoken languages in the world, yet it is not known how
stuttering manifests in English-Mandarin bilinguals. In this
research, the authors investigated whether the severity and
type of stuttering is different in English and Mandarin in
English-Mandarin bilinguals, and whether this difference was
influenced by language dominance. METHOD: Thirty
English-Mandarin bilinguals who stutter (BWS), ages 12-44
years, were categorized into 3 groups (15 English-dominant, 4
Mandarin-dominant, and 11 balanced bilinguals) using a
self-report classification tool. Three 10-min conversations in
English and Mandarin were assessed by 2 English-Mandarin
bilingual clinicians for percent syllables stuttered (%SS),
perceived stuttering severity (SEV), and types of stuttering
behaviors using the Lidcombe Behavioral Data Language (LBDL;
Packman & Onslow, 1998; Teesson, Packman, & Onslow,
2003). RESULTS: English-dominant and Mandarin-dominant BWS
exhibited higher %SS and SEV scores in their less dominant
language, whereas the scores for the balanced bilinguals were
similar for both languages. The difference in the percentage of
stutters per LBDL category between English and Mandarin was not
markedly different for any bilingual group. CONCLUSIONS:
Language dominance appeared to influence the severity but not
the types of stuttering behaviors in BWS. Clinicians working
with BWS need to assess language dominance when diagnosing
stuttering severity in bilingual clients.
September 2008
The Peer Attitudes Toward
Children who Stutter (PATCS) scale: an evaluation of validity,
reliability and the negativity of attitudes.
Langevin M, Kleitman S, Packman A,
Onslow M.
Australian Stuttering Research
Centre.
Background: Persistent calls for
school-based education about stuttering necessitate a better
understanding of peer attitudes toward children who stutter and
a means to measure outcomes of such educational interventions.
Langevin and Hagler in 2004 developed the Peer Attitudes Toward
Children who Stutter scale (PATCS) to address these needs and
gave preliminary evidence of reliability and construct
validity. Aims: To examine further the psychometric properties
of PATCS and to examine the negativity of attitudes. Methods
& Procedures: PATCS was administered to 760 Canadian
children in grades 3-6. Measures included reliability, a
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), a known groups analysis,
convergent validity with the Pro-Victim Scale of Rigby and
Slee, and the negativity of attitudes. Outcomes & Results:
PATCS appears to tap a second-order general attitude factor and
three first-order factors representing the constructs of
Positive Social Distance (PSD), Social Pressure (SP), and
Verbal Interaction (VI). In the known groups analysis,
participants who had contact with someone who stutters had
higher scores (more positive attitudes) than those who had not,
and girls had higher scores than boys. PATCS correlated
moderately (0.43, p<0.01) with the Pro-Victim scale.
Finally, one-fifth (21.7%) of participants had scores that were
somewhat to very negative. Conclusions & Implications:
Results provide evidence of the validity and reliability of
PATCS and confirm the need for school-based education about
stuttering. The PSD and SP factors suggest that education
include discussions about (1) similarities and differences
among children who do and do not stutter in order to increase
acceptance, and (2) making personal choices and handling peer
pressure in thinking about children who stutter. The VI factor
suggests that open discussion about stuttering may alleviate
frustration experienced by listeners and provide the
opportunity to give strategies for responding appropriately.
Results also suggest that education involve contact with a
person who stutters.
The effects of bilingualism
on stuttering during late childhood.
Howell P, Davis SR, Williams R.
University College London, United
Kingdom.
OBJECTIVES: To document distinct patterns
of language use by bilingual children (use of an alternative
language exclusively, LE, or along with English, BIL). To
establish how these patterns affect onset of stuttering, school
performance and recovery rate relative to monolingual speakers
who stutter (MONO). DESIGN: Clinical referral sample with cases
classified by speech-language therapists. Supplementary data
obtained from speech recordings, interviews with child and
family. SETTING: South-East England, 1999-2007. Participants
Children aged 8-12 plus who stuttered (monolingual and
bilingual) and fluent bilingual controls (FB). MAIN OUTCOME
MEASURES: Participants' stuttering history, SATS scores,
measures of recovery or persistence of stuttering. RESULTS: The
sample of 317 children had 69 bilinguals (prevalence rate of
bilingualism in the stuttering sample was 21.8%). 38 children
used a language other than English primarily or exclusively in
the home and 36 of these (94.7%) bilinguals who stuttered did
so in both their languages. There were fewer LE than BIL
stuttering children at time of first referral to clinic (of the
bilinguals who stuttered, 15/38, 39.5%, were LE and 23/38,
60.5%, were BIL). The reverse was the case in the fluent
control sample (of the bilinguals who did not stutter, 28/38,
73.7%, were LE and 10/38, 26.3%, were BIL). The association
between stuttering and bilingual group (LE/BIL) was significant
by 2 and this is consistent with a higher chance of stuttering
for BIL than LE speakers. For speakers who stuttered, age of
stuttering onset for LE and BIL was similar to that reported
for MONO groups (4 years 9 months, 4 years 10 months and 4
years 3 months for LE, BIL, MONO respectively) and males were
affected in each of these groups to about the same extent (the
male/female ratio was 4.1:1, 4.75:1 and 4.43:1 for LE, BIL and
MONO respectively). For the 29 children who were old enough to
complete the assessments, educational achievement at key stages
one and two was not affected by either form of bilingualism
relative to the MONO and fluent bilingual groups. For these
same children, recovery rate for LE and MONO controls was
significantly higher by 2 than for those who were BIL since
birth (recovery rate for LE and MONO together was 55%, and for
BIL was 25%). CONCLUSIONS: There was an increased chance of
stuttering onset for BIL children. The chances of recovery from
stuttering were lower for BIL speakers than for LE and MONO
speakers.
Persistent developmental
stuttering as a cortical-subcortical dysfunction: evidence from
muscle activation.
de Andrade CR, Sassi FC, < Juste F,
de Mendonça LI.
Departament of Physiotherapy,
Speech-Language and Hearing Sciences and Occupational Therapy,
School of Medicine, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, SP,
Brazil. clauan@usp.br
BACKGROUND: One contemporary view of
stuttering posits that speech disfluencies arise from anomalous
speech motor control. PURPOSE: To verify the rest muscle
tension and speech reaction time of fluent and stuttering
adults. METHOD: 22 adults, divided in two groups: G1--11 fluent
individuals; G2--11 stutterers. Electromyography recordings
(inferior orbicularis oris) were collected in two different
situations: during rest and in a reaction time activity.
RESULTS: The groups were significantly different considering
rest muscle tension (G2 higher recordings) and did not differ
when considering speech reaction time and muscle activity
during speech. There was a strong positive correlation between
speech reaction time and speech muscle activity for G2--the
longer the speech reaction time, the higher the muscle activity
during speech. CONCLUSION: In addition to perceptible episodes
of speech disfluency, stutterers exhibit anomalies in speech
motor output during fluent speech. Correlations with a possible
cortical-subcortical disorder are discussed.
The speech naturalness of
people who stutter speaking under delayed auditory feedback as
perceived by different groups of listeners.
Van Borsel J, Eeckhout H.
Ghent University Hospital, Belgium.
john.vanborsel@ugent.be
This study investigated listeners'
perception of the speech naturalness of people who stutter
(PWS) speaking under delayed auditory feedback (DAF) with
particular attention for possible listener differences. Three
panels of judges consisting of 14 stuttering individuals, 14
speech language pathologists, and 14 naive listeners rated the
naturalness of speech samples of stuttering and non-stuttering
individuals using a 9-point interval scale. Results clearly
indicate that these three groups evaluate naturalness
differently. Naive listeners appear to be more severe in their
judgements than speech language pathologists and stuttering
listeners, and speech language pathologists are apparently more
severe than PWS. The three listener groups showed similar
trends with respect to the relationship between speech
naturalness and speech rate. Results of all three indicated
that for PWS, the slower a speaker's rate was, the less natural
speech was judged to sound. The three listener groups also
showed similar trends with regard to naturalness of the
stuttering versus the non-stuttering individuals. All three
panels considered the speech of the non-stuttering participants
more natural. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able
to: (1) discuss the speech naturalness of people who stutter
speaking under delayed auditory feedback, (2) discuss listener
differences about the naturalness of people who stutter
speaking under delayed auditory feedback, and (3) discuss the
importance of speech rate for the naturalness of speech.
Influences of rate, length,
and complexity on speech disfluency in a single-speech sample
in preschool children who stutter.
Sawyer J, Chon H, Ambrose NG.
Department of Communication Sciences and
Disorders, Illinois State University, 204 Fairchild Hall,
Campus Box 4720, Normal, IL 61790-4720, USA.
jsawyer@ilstu.edu
The purpose of the present study was (1) to
determine whether speech rate, utterance length, and
grammatical complexity (number of clauses and clausal
constituents per utterance) influenced stuttering-like
disfluencies as children became more disfluent at the end of a
1200-syllable speech sample [Sawyer, J., & Yairi, E.
(2006). The effect of sample size on the assessment of
stuttering severity. American Journal of Speech-Language
Pathology, 15, 36-44] and (2) to explore the interaction of
speech rate, length, and grammatical complexity at the
beginning (syllables 1-300, Section A) and the end (syllables
901-1200, Section B) of the speech sample. Participants were
eight boys and six girls (M=40.9 months) who were selected from
the Sawyer and Yairi [Sawyer, J., & Yairi, E. (2006). The
effect of sample size on the assessment of stuttering severity.
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 36-44]
study. Mean length of utterance (MLU) in morphemes, the number
of clauses, clausal constituents, and articulation rate,
measured in syllables per second were analyzed from the
children's conversational speech. The median split procedure
[Logan, K., & Conture, E. (1995). Length, grammatical
complexity, and rate differences in stuttered and fluent
conversational utterances of children who stutter. Journal of
Fluency Disorders, 20, 35-61; Yaruss, J. S. (1997). Utterance
timing and childhood stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders,
22, 263-286] was used to study interactions between
articulation rate, utterance length, and grammatical complexity
across the two sections. The mean number of clauses per
utterance, clausal constituents per utterance, and articulation
rate revealed no significant differences between Section A and
Section B, whereas MLU significantly increased in Section B
(p=.013). Clausal constituents and MLU were significantly
correlated both in Sections A and B. The median split procedure
revealed trends for utterances characterized as high length and
low-speech rate to be greater in number in Section B than A,
but the differences were not significant. EDUCATIONAL
OBJECTIVES: The reader will learn about and be able to: (a)
describe the influence of grammatical complexity and mean
length of utterance on disfluent speech; (b) compare different
procedures for assessing speech rate and determine why the
effects of articulation rate have been inconclusive; (c)
discuss procedures for comparing length, rate, and complexity
across a single-speech sample; and (d) explain why therapeutic
methods that emphasize shorter utterance lengths, rather than
only slower speech rates, are advisable in establishing fluency
in preschool children who stutter.
The effect of stuttering
measurement training on judging stuttering occurrence in
preschool children who stutter.
Einarsdóttir J, Ingham RJ.
University of Iceland, Reykjavik,
Iceland. jeinars@hi.is
PURPOSE: To evaluate the efficacy of a
standardized training program to improve preschool teachers'
ability to identify occurrences of stuttering accurately and
reliably in preschool children who stutter (CWS). METHOD: An
Icelandic version of the Stuttering Measurement Assessment and
Training (SMAAT) program [Ingham, R. J., Cordes, A. K., Kilgo,
M., & Moglia, R. (1998). Stuttering measurement assessment
and training (SMAAT). Santa Barbara, CA: University of
California, Santa Barbara] was developed using 2-min
audio-visual recordings of nine Icelandic-speaking CWS (3-5
years). Twenty preschool teachers from preschools in Iceland
volunteered to participate and were randomly allocated to an
experimental and control group. The preschool teachers judged
stuttering on nine pre-judged, interval-classified speech
samples on two occasions 2-3 weeks apart; only the experimental
group received judgment training between Occasion 1 and 2.
RESULTS: The experimental and control groups displayed, on
average, above 80% accuracy in identifying stuttering in the
sampled CWS, even prior to training. After training the
experimental group showed significantly higher mean percent
judgment accuracy (p<.01), while the control group's mean
accuracy level showed no significant difference between
Occasion 1 and 2. CONCLUSIONS: The interval-based training
program did improve the accuracy with which the judges
identified stuttering in preschool CWS. However, the findings
also showed that the preschool teachers possessed relatively
satisfactory stuttering judgment skills, even without training.
EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: The reader will be able to (a) explain
the development of a procedure for establishing standardized
training material for measuring and identifying stuttering in
preschool children, (b) evaluate whether preschool teachers are
generally accurate judges of stuttering in young children, and
(c) describe how their stuttering judgment accuracy can be
improved through training.
October
2008
Results from applying the
monaural compressed speech test in Spanish at 75% and 100% in
cases of stuttering and controls
Peñaloza-López YR, Téllez G, Pérez-Ruiz
S, Silva MJ, García-Pedroza F.
Area de Procesos Centrales de la
Audición y de Foniatría, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de
México, México DF, México. yploza@yahoo.com.mx
SUMMARY AIM: To evaluate the results
obtained from applying the monaural compressed speech test in
Spanish at 75% and 100% in cases of stutterers and controls.
SUBJECTS AND METHODS: The study involved 50 cases with ages
ranging from 8 to 36 years, 25 of whom were stutterers and 25
controls, who were paired one to one by age, sex and schooling.
Severe cases of stuttering were not included. Subjects had to
have normal auditory functioning, which was evaluated by means
of audiometry and peripheral vocal audiometry, and also normal
cognitive functioning, which was assessed using TONI-2 in
children and by the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State
Examination in adults. RESULTS: The average number of correct
answers on the CSTS at 75% was 60.98% in cases of stutterers
and 82.04% in the group of controls (Student's t = -7.4; p =
0.0001), and on the CSTS at 100% the figures were 56.56% and
73.16%, respectively, (Student's t = -5.9; p = 0.0001).
Sensitivity and specificity were calculated for each test by
means of ROC curves, together with the cut-off point.
CONCLUSION: Applying the CSTS in individuals who stutter helped
us to identify disorders in the temporal processing of auditory
information.
September
2008
Unhelpful thoughts and
beliefs linked to social anxiety in stuttering: development of
a measure.
St Clare T, Menzies RG, Onslow M,
Packman A, Thompson R, Block S.
School of Behavioural and Community
Health Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney,
Australia.
Background: Those who stutter have a
proclivity to social anxiety. Yet, to date, there is no
comprehensive measure of thoughts and beliefs about stuttering
that represent the cognitions associated with that anxiety.
Aims: The present paper describes the development of a measure
to assess unhelpful thoughts and beliefs about stuttering.
Methods & Procedures: The Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs
about Stuttering (UTBAS) self-report measure contains 66 items
that assess the frequency of unhelpful thoughts and beliefs.
Items were constructed from a comprehensive file audit of all
stuttering cases seen in a cognitive-behavior therapy based
treatment programme over a ten-year period. Outcomes &
Results: Preliminary investigations indicate that the UTBAS has
high levels of test-retest reliability (r = 0.89) and internal
consistency (Chronbach's alpha = 0.98). It has good
known-groups validity, being able to discriminate between
stuttering and non-stuttering participants on items that
contain no reference to stuttering [t(38) = 8.06, p<0.0001],
with a large effect size (d = 2.3). It has good convergent
validity (r = 0.53-0.72) and discriminant validity (r =
0.24-0.27). The UTBAS sensitivity to change was supported by
improvements in thoughts and beliefs related to social anxiety
following cognitive-behavioural treatment for anxiety in
stuttering [t(25) = 10.13, p<0.0001]. The effect size was
large (d = 2.5). Conclusions & Implications: Implications
for the use of the UTBAS as an outcome measure and a clinical
tool are discussed, along with the potential value of the UTBAS
to explore the well-documented social anxiety experienced by
those who stutter.
August
2008
Identification of children's
stuttered and nonstuttered speech by highly experienced judges:
binary judgments and comparisons with disfluency-types
definitions.
Bothe AK.
Department of Communication Sciences and
Special Education, The University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602,
USA. abothe@uga.edu
PURPOSE: The purposes of this study were
(a) to determine whether highly experienced clinicians and
researchers agreed with each other in judging the presence or
absence of stuttering in the speech of children who stutter and
(b) to determine how those binary stuttered/nonstuttered
judgments related to categorizations of the same speech based
on disfluency-types descriptions of stuttering. METHOD: Eleven
highly experienced judges made binary judgments of the presence
or absence of stuttering for 600 audiovisually recorded 5-s
speech samples from twenty 2- to 8-year-old children who
stuttered. These judgments were compared with each other and
with disfluency-types judgments in multiple
interval-by-interval assessments and by using multiple
definitions of agreement. RESULTS: Interjudge agreement for the
highly experienced judges in the binary stuttered/nonstuttered
task varied from 39.0% to 89.1%, depending on methods and
definitions used. Congruence between binary judgments and
categorizations based on disfluency types also varied depending
on methods and definitions, from 21.6% to 100%. CONCLUSIONS:
Agreement among highly experienced judges, and congruence
between their binary judgments of stuttering and
categorizations based on disfluency types, were relatively high
using some definitions and very low using others. These results
suggest the use of measurement methods other than those based
on disfluency types for quantifying or describing children's
stuttering. They also suggest both the need for, and potential
methods for, training to increase judges' accuracy and
agreement in identifying children's stuttering.
July 2008
Measurement of Phonated
Intervals During Four Fluency-Inducing Conditions.
Davidow JH, Bothe AK, Andreatta RD,
Ye J.
Hofstra University.
PURPOSE: Previous investigations of persons
who stutter have demonstrated changes in vocalization variables
during fluency-inducing conditions (FICs). A series of studies
has also shown that a reduction in short intervals of
phonation, those from 30-200 milliseconds (ms), is associated
with decreased stuttering. The purpose of this study,
therefore, was to test the hypothesis that the distribution of
phonated intervals (PIs) should change during four of the most
well-known FICs. METHOD: A repeated-measures design was used to
explore the relationship between Pis and stuttering during four
FICs: chorus reading, prolonged speech, singing, and rhythmic
stimulation. Most conditions were conducted at two different
speech rates. The distribution of PIs was measured during these
conditions and compared to PI distributions obtained during
control conditions. RESULTS: Overall PI distributions were
significantly different during all four FICs, as compared to
control conditions. It was also found that PIs in the range of
30-150 ms were reduced across all FICs, at all speech rates.
CONCLUSION: These results provide further evidence of the
importance of phonation variables to our understanding of how
FICs may operate, and their importance to the treatment of
stuttering. These findings, along with previous studies that
showed how purposefully reducing the number of short PIs
resulted in the elimination of stuttering, suggest that
treatment programs based on prolonged speech or PIs, in
particular, may benefit from emphasizing a reduction in the
number of short phonated intervals and a simultaneous increase
in the number of longer PIs.
A nationwide survey of
nonspeech oral motor exercise use: implications for
evidence-based practice.
Lof GL, Watson MM.
MGH Institute of Health Professions,
Boston, MA 02129-4557, USA. glof@mghihp.edu
PURPOSE: A nationwide survey was conducted
to determine if speech-language pathologists (SLPs) use
nonspeech oral motor exercises (NSOMEs) to address children's
speech sound problems. For those SLPs who used NSOMEs, the
survey also identified (a) the types of NSOMEs used by the
SLPs, (b) the SLPs' underlying beliefs about why they use
NSOMEs, (c) clinicians' training for these exercises, (d) the
application of NSOMEs across various clinical populations, and
(e) specific tasks/procedures/tools that are used for
intervention. METHOD: A total of 2,000 surveys were mailed to a
randomly selected subgroup of SLPs, obtained from the American
Speech-Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) membership roster,
who self-identified that they worked in various settings with
children who have speech sound problems. The questions required
answers that used both a forced choice and Likert-type scales.
RESULTS: The response rate was 27.5% (537 out of 2,000). Of
these respondents, 85% reported using NSOMEs to deal with
children's speech sound production problems. Those SLPs
reported that the research literature supports the use of
NSOMEs, and that they learned to use these techniques from
continuing education events. They also stated that NSOMEs can
help improve the speech of children from disparate etiologies,
and "warming up" and strengthening the articulators are
important components of speech sound therapy. CONCLUSION: There
are theoretical and research data that challenge both the use
of NSOMEs and the efficacy of such exercises in resolving
speech sound problems. SLPs need to follow the concepts of
evidence-based practice in order to determine if these
exercises are actually effective in bringing about changes in
speech productions.
Diagnostics and therapy of
stuttering children
Brosch S, Winkler S.
Sektion für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie
der Abteilung für Hals-Nasen-Ohrenheilkunde, Universität Ulm.
sibylle.brosch@uniklinik-ulm.de
We demonstrate an overall concept for the
early diagnosis and therapy of stuttering in early childhood.
The idiographic view of childhood stuttering is detailed and a
screening instrument presented to help decide between normal
development of fluent speech and actual stuttering and hence
whether referral is appropriate or not. The previously held
dictum that the stuttering preschool child should be left alone
is wrong - early diagnosis and intervention are important. The
article shows that direct intervention of stuttering in
preschool children is possible and does make sense. Because the
prognosis for fluent speech diminishes rapidly from as early as
age eight, the risk factors for chronic stuttering must be
found and early therapy begun.
June 2008
Late childhood
stuttering.
Howell P, Davis S, Williams R.
Department of Psychology, University
College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, England.
p.howell@ucl.ac.uk
PURPOSE: A study was conducted that
examined factors that lead children who stutter at around age 8
years to persist in the disorder when they reach age 12 years.
METHOD: Seventy-six children were verified to be stuttering at
initial assessment. When they reached 12 years of age, they
were classified as persistent or recovered. A range of measures
was taken at the 2 age points, and measures were examined by
recovery group. RESULTS: Although the tendency for more males
than females to stutter was confirmed, the reasons for this
tendency are not apparent for these speakers. Different
patterns in speech were observed: Severity ratings of the
recovered speakers dropped by age 12+. The severity ratings for
the persistent speakers remained high at 12+, and dysfluency
types tended to change from whole words to part words.
Persistent and recovered speakers differed on temperamental
performance at around age 8 years and performed differently on
sensory and motor tasks at age 12+ years. CONCLUSIONS:
Stuttering in late childhood affects mainly males. The later a
child attends clinic, the longer he or she will stutter. Speech
patterns of children who persist diverge from those who recover
or who are fluent. As speakers persist, there are
temperamental, sensory, and motor changes.
May
2008
Stuttering: an
overview.
Prasse JE, Kikano GE.
Stamford Hospital, Stamford,
Connecticut, USA. Jprasse@stamhealth.org
Speech dysfluency (stuttering) is common in
children. Although stuttering often resolves before adulthood,
it can cause significant anxiety for children and their
families. Stuttering speech patterns are often easily
identifiable; when a child is learning to talk, repetition of
sounds or words, prolonged pauses, or excessively long sounds
in words usually occur. Secondary behaviors (e.g., eye
blinking, jaw jerking, involuntary head or other movements)
that accompany stuttering can further embarrass the child,
leading to a fear of speaking. The etiology of stuttering is
controversial, but contributing factors may include cognitive
abilities, genetics, sex of the child, and environmental
influences. Research has shown that more than 80 percent of
stuttering cases are classified as developmental problems,
although stuttering can also be classified as a neurologic or,
less commonly, psychogenic problem. The initial assessment of
patients who stutter addresses the severity of dysfluency;
secondary behaviors; and the impact of stuttering, such as
patient distress. Further testing is useful in assessing the
need for therapy. Pharmacologic therapy has not been shown to
improve stuttering. Encouraging patients to talk slowly and the
use of fluency-shaping mechanisms such as delayed auditory
feedback devices to slow the speech rate can help minimize or
eliminate stuttering. For patients with persistent stuttering,
controlled fluency or stuttering modification therapy may be
effective.
Comparisons of audio and
audiovisual measures of stuttering frequency and severity in
preschool-age children.
Rousseau I, Onslow M, Packman A,
Jones M.
Australian Stuttering Research Centre,
The University of Sydney, Australia.
PURPOSE: To determine whether measures of
stuttering frequency and measures of overall stuttering
severity in preschoolers differ when made from audio-only
recordings compared with audiovisual recordings. METHOD: Four
blinded speech-language pathologists who had extensive
experience with preschoolers who stutter measured stuttering
frequency and rated overall severity from audio-only and
audiovisual recordings of 36 preschool children who were
stuttering. Stuttering frequency (percentage of syllables
stuttered [%SS]) was based on counts of perceptually
unambiguous stutterings, made in real time, and overall
severity was measured using a 9-point rating scale. RESULTS:
Stuttering frequency was statistically significantly lower by
around 20% when made from audio-only recordings. This was found
to be directly attributable to differences in the counts of
stuttered syllables, rather than to differences in the total
numbers of syllables spoken. No significant differences were
found between recording modalities for the ratings of overall
severity. Correlations between %SS scores in the 2 modalities
and severity rating scores in the 2 modalities were high,
indicating that observers agreed on data trends across speech
samples. CONCLUSIONS: Measures of %SS made from audio-only
recordings may underestimate stuttering frequency in
preschoolers. Although audio-only %SS measures may
underestimate stuttering frequency at the start of a clinical
trial to a clinically significant extent, posttreatment scores
at or below 1.0%SS are likely to underestimate by 0.2%SS or
less, which is clinically insignificant.
Multiple voices in Charles
Van Riper's desensitization therapy.
Leahy MM.
Clinical Speech & Language Studies,
Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland. mleahy@tcd.ie
BACKGROUND: Charles Van Riper (1905-94)
author, researcher and practitioner, was one of the major
contributors to the field of stuttering in the 20th century.
His series of Action Therapy videotapes provide a useful model
of how therapy was implemented by a master clinician. AIMS: Van
Riper's session with a client is analysed to reveal how voices
are used by these two participants during desensitization
therapy, and to demonstrate how Van Riper's expertise manifests
itself during the session. METHODS & PROCEDURES:
Interaction between Van Riper and a client during the
desensitization stage of therapy was transcribed, annotated,
and analysed using Goffman's (1974) frame analysis. Elements in
the extensive discourse (about 4800 words exchanged) were
chosen to illustrate a range of distinctive voices used by Van
Riper and the client during this 'crucial' stage of therapy.
OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Van Riper shifts his voice throughout
the session. His comments are sometimes directed to the viewing
audience, at which times he tends to use the voice of an
exegeter. When directing his comments to the client, he uses
two voices: an authoritative one and one that aligns with the
person's feelings. The voice of the client is often silenced,
especially when Van Riper is being authoritative. Sometimes Van
Riper uses his expertise by speaking through the voice of the
client. This is especially true when summarizing what the
client might be feeling. The true voice of the client emerges
only occasionally. Detailed discourse analysis focusing on the
roles played by participants as revealed through footings taken
by speakers show complexity in the interaction that is not
obvious at surface level. CONCLUSIONS: By using discourse
analysis, the multiple voices that Van Riper uses to achieve
the complex tasks involved in desensitization can be
specified.
Investigating factors related
to the effects of time-out on stuttering in adults.
Franklin DE, Taylor CL, Hennessey NW,
Beilby JM.
School of Psychology, Curtin University
of Technology, GPO Box U1987, Perth, Western Australia 6845.
cliane.franklin@hotmail.com
BACKGROUND: Response-contingent time-out
has been shown to be an effective technique for enhancing
fluency in people who stutter. However, the factors that
determine individual responsiveness to time-out are not well
understood. AIMS: The study investigated the effectiveness of
using response-contingent time-out to reduce stuttering
frequency in adults who stutter. In addition, it investigated
the predictive value of participants' stutter severity, age,
previous treatment history, and type of stutter on the
responsiveness to time-out conditioning. METHODS &
PROCEDURES: Sixty people who stutter participated in the study.
Half were exposed to time-out following each moment of
stuttering over a 40-min period, the remaining participants
acted as controls. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: Results showed that
individuals who stutter are highly responsive to time-out, and
that the participants with a more severe stutter responded
better than those with a mild stutter. To a lesser degree,
previous treatment and speech rate also influenced treatment
success. Age and type of stutter did not, although the
proportion of repetition types of stutters increased over the
experiment conditions, with prolongations and blocks
decreasing, for the treatment group. CONCLUSIONS: This simple
operant conditioning treatment method is effective in reducing
stuttering. Individuals respond to time-out regardless of their
age, type of stutter, stuttering severity or treatment history,
thus it is a treatment methodology potentially suitable for all
clients. This study investigated initial responsiveness to
time-out; therefore, further research is necessary to determine
the durability of fluency over time.
University students'
perceptions of pre-school and kindergarten children who
stutter.
Betz IR, Blood GW, Blood IM.
Diamond Elementary School, MD, United
States.
To determine how early "the stuttering
stereotype" is assigned, 160 university students rated a
hypothetical vignette depicting either a 3-, 4-, 5-, or
6-year-old with or without the statement "He stutters". A
factor analysis of the semantic differential scale showed a
three-factor solution comprised of 17 of the 25 bi-polar
adjective pairs. The factor labeled personality showed
significantly more negative ratings for 2-, 4-, 5-, or
6-year-old children based on the inclusion of the "He stutters"
sentence. There were no differences between male and female
raters. A significant difference was found between raters who
were knew someone who stuttered and raters who did not know
someone who stuttered on the personality factor. Raters who
were knew someone who stuttered were significantly more likely
to assign more positive ratings to the children. LEARNING
OUTCOMES: Readers should be able to learn and understand: (1)
the research describing the negative stereotypes associated
with stuttering; (2) the vignette method used to evaluate
stereotypes in children and youth; (3) the negative perceptions
of the sentence "He stutters" on raters' perception of
personality, sociability and speech for children as young as
3-, 4-, 5-, or 6-year-olds; and (4) the familiarity with a
person who stutters and raters' perceptions of children who
stutter.
Fluency shaping with surface
electromyography: a pilot study
Furquim de Andrade CR, Sassi FC, Juste
FS, Ercolin B.
Departamento de Fisioterapia,
Fonoaudiologia e Terapia Ocupacional, Faculdade de Medicina,
Universidade de São Paulo. clauan@usp.br
BACKGROUND: the use of a technological
resource in fluency promotion. AIM: to verify the effectiveness
of a stuttering treatment based on the use of surface
electromyography (SEMG) exclusively. METHOD: participants were
four stuttering adults of both genders. Assessment, pre and
post-treatment, consisted of a speech gathering session and the
analyses of the rest tension and of the reaction time for
speech. Treatment consisted of twelve twenty minute training
sessions monitored by SEMG. RESULTS: there was a statistically
significant reduction in the number of stuttering-like
disfluencies (p = 0.094) and in the number of other
disfluencies (p = 0.014). The other parameters, as well as
differences in the electromyographic measurements, did not
present significant variation. CONCLUSION: SEMG proved to be
effective in the reduction of stuttering, with no need of
association to other techniques.
April
2008
Effects of stuttering
severity and therapy involvement on role entrapment of people
who stutter.
Gabel RM, Hughes S, Daniels D.
Department of Communication Disorders,
Bowling Green State University, Bowling Green, OH 43403, USA.
rgabel@bgnet.bgsu.edu
The primary purpose of this study was to
examine whether a group of university students would report
role entrapment of people who stutter (PWS) in the form of
occupational stereotyping. The study also examined whether
severity of stuttering (mild or severe) and level of therapy
involvement (choosing or not choosing to attend therapy)
affected the perceptions of role entrapment. To examine these
issues, 260 students completed the Vocational Advice Scale
(VAS) [Gabel, R. M., Blood, G. W., Tellis, G., & Althouse,
M. T. (2004). Measuring role entrapment of people who stutter.
Journal of Fluency Disorders, 29, 27-49]. Results suggested
that stuttering severity and the level of therapy involvement
did not appear alter the judges' reports for all of the careers
except for the career of speech therapist. For the career of
speech therapist, therapy involvement improved the
participants' reports and stuttering severity had no effect.
Additionally, findings suggested that university students
reported that 16 of the careers listed on the VAS were
appropriate choices for people who stutter and were less
certain about advising for 27 of the careers. Thus, the
findings from this study do not support the notion that
stuttering leads to role entrapment in the form vocational
stereotyping and variations in therapy involvement or
stuttering severity do not change perceptions of role
entrapment. LEARNER OUTCOMES: The reader will be able to (1)
identify common stereotypes of PWS, (2) describe the possible
effects of stereotyping and role entrapment, and (3) describe
the effects of severity and therapy involvement of role
entrapment of PWS.
March
2008
Health and human services for
persons who stutter and education of logopedists in
East-European countries.
Fibiger S, Peters HF, Euler HA,
Neumann K.
Rehabilitation Centre, Region of
Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark.
Steen.Fibiger@soc.regionsyddanmark.dk
The International Association of Logopedics
and Phoniatrics (IALP) assessed the therapy status of fluency
disorders, service opportunities, and education of logopedists
(speech-language pathologists) with a mail survey in Eastern
Europe. Information was collected on the following aspects:
incidence, prevalence, availability of information,
non-therapeutic support for persons who stutter (PWS),
providers of diagnostics and therapy, cooperating
professionals, therapy approaches, forms, goals, financing,
early detection and prevention, training of professionals,
specialization in stuttering therapy, needs for improving the
situation of PWS, and problems which hinder better care.
Stuttering therapy for children is available in many countries
and is frequently provided by the educational system. Therapy
for adults is provided best by the health services but is not
satisfactorily available everywhere. Modern therapeutic
approaches coexist with obsolete ones. Lack of resources,
awareness, entitlement, and assessment of therapy effectiveness
are pervasive problems. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES: Readers will be
able to describe and evaluate: (1) the therapy status of
fluency disorders and service opportunities in various
East-European countries; (2) the training of logopedists
(speech-language pathologists); (3) specialization in
stuttering therapy; and (4) the organizational services for PWS
within the health and human service systems.
Investigating speech motor
practice and learning in people who stutter.
Namasivayam AK, van Lieshout P.
Oral Dynamics Laboratory, Department of
Speech-Language Pathology, University of Toronto, 160-500
University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1V7.
a.namasivayam@utoronto.ca
In this exploratory study, we investigated
whether or not people who stutter (PWS) show motor practice and
learning changes similar to those of people who do not stutter
(PNS). To this end, five PWS and five PNS repeated a set of
non-words at two different rates (normal and fast) across three
test sessions (T1, T2 on the same day and T3 on a separate day,
at least 1 week apart). The results indicated that PWS and PNS
may resemble each other on a number of performance variables
(such as movement amplitude and duration), but they differ in
terms of practice and learning on variables that relate to
movement stability and strength of coordination patterns. These
findings are interpreted in support of recent claims about
speech motor skill limitations in PWS. EDUCATIONAL OBJECTIVES:
The reader will be able to: (1) define oral articulatory
changes associated with motor practice and learning and their
measurement; (2) summarize findings from previous studies
examining motor practice and learning in PWS; and (3) discuss
hypotheses that could account for the present findings that
suggest PWS and PNS differ in their speech motor learning
abilities.
January
2008
Relationship between
stuttering severity in children and their mothers speaking
rate.
Dehqan A, Bakhtiar M, Panahi SS,
Ashayeri H.
Department of Speech Therapy, School of
Paramedicine, Zahedan University of Medical Sciences, Zahedan,
Iran. dehqan@zdmu.ac.ir
CONTEXT AND OBJECTIVE: Stuttering is a
complex disease that influences occupational, social, academic
and emotional achievements. The aim of this study was to
correlate the stuttering severity index with speaking rates of
mothers and children. DESIGN AND SETTING: Cross-sectional
study, at the child rehabilitation clinics of Tehran city.
METHODS: 35 pairs of mothers and their children who stuttered
were studied. There were 29 boys and six girls, of mean age 8.5
years (range: 5.1-12.0). Speech samples from the mother-child
pairs were audiotaped for approximately 15 minutes, until a
reciprocal verbal interaction had been obtained. This sample
was then analyzed in accordance with a stuttering severity
index test and speaking rate parameters. RESULTS: The research
results outlined a significant relationship between the mothers
speaking rate and their childrens stuttering severity.
CONCLUSION: The results suggest that the mothers speaking rate
should be incorporated in the assessment and treatment of
stuttering.
Judging stuttering in an
unfamiliar language: the importance of closeness to the native
language.
Van Borsel J, Leahy MM, Pereira MB.
Ghent University Hospital, Ghent,
Belgium. john.vanborsel@ugent.be
In order to test the hypothesis that
closeness to the listener's native language is a determining
factor when identifying stuttering in an unfamiliar language,
three panels of different linguistic background were asked to
make judgements of stuttering in a sample of Dutch speakers. It
was found that a panel speaking Dutch and a panel speaking
English (both West Germanic languages) performed better in
identifying Dutch people who stutter and people who do not
stutter than a panel speaking Brazilian Portuguese (a Romance
language) thus confirming the existence of a closeness of
language influence. Further analysis showed that when the
native language is more remote from the unfamiliar language
there is the possibility of a higher risk for false positive
identification.
Perceived stigmatization of
children with speech-language impairment and their
parents.
Macharey G, von Suchodoletz W.
Department of Developmental Disorders,
Clinic of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and
Psychotherapy, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich,
Germany.
OBJECTIVE: Developmental disorders in
childhood are generally assumed to have stigmatizing effects.
The goal of the present study was to assess whether parents of
children with speech-language impairment perceive
stigmatization of their child or themselves and which variables
influence the degree of negative labeling. SUBJECTS AND
METHODS: The study was based on 362 questionnaires completed by
parents of children with speech-language impairment. The
questionnaires concerned perceived stigmatization by other
children, other adults and family members as a result of the
child's developmental problems. RESULTS: In our sample, about
50% of the parents reported negative labeling of their child
and about 30% felt they were involved in the stigmatizing
process. Parents whose children also had behavioral problems
more often reported negative labeling than parents whose
children did not. CONCLUSION: The findings suggest that parents
of children with speech-language disorders often perceive
stigmatization of their children or themselves. In counseling
such families, professionals should therefore address
stigmatization and its consequences as a separate and important
issue.
Dissecting choral speech:
properties of the accompanist critical to stuttering
reduction.
Kiefte M, Armson J.
School of Human Communication Disorders,
Dalhousie University, Nova Scota, Canada. mkiefte@dal.ca
The effects of choral speech and altered
auditory feedback (AAF) on stuttering frequency were compared
to identify those properties of choral speech that make it a
more effective condition for stuttering reduction. Seventeen
adults who stutter (AWS) participated in an experiment
consisting of special choral speech conditions that were
manipulated to selectively eliminate specific differences
between choral speech and AAF. Consistent with previous
findings, results showed that both choral speech and AAF
reduced stuttering compared to solo reading. Although
reductions under AAF were substantial, they were less dramatic
than those for choral speech. Stuttering reduction for choral
speech was highly robust even when the accompanist's voice
temporally lagged that of the AWS, when there was no
opportunity for dynamic interplay between the AWS and
accompanist, and when the accompanist was replaced by the AWS's
own voice, all of which approximate specific features of AAF.
Choral speech was also highly effective in reducing stuttering
across changes in speech rate and for both familiar and
unfamiliar passages. We concluded that differences in
properties between choral speech and AAF other than those that
were manipulated in this experiment must account for
differences in stuttering reduction. LEARNING OUTCOMES: The
reader will be able to (1) describe differences in stuttering
reduction associated with altered auditory feedback compared to
choral speech conditions and (2) describe differences between
delivery of a second voice signal as an altered rendition of
the speakers own voice (altered auditory feedback) and
alterations in the voice of an accompanist (choral speech).
Telerehabilitation for
service delivery in speech-language pathology.
Theodoros DG.
Division of Speech Pathology, School of
Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Queensland,
St Lucia 4072, Australia. d.theodoros@uq.edu.au
Communication disorders in adults and
children can have a significant effect on their quality of life
and on that of their families. Speech-language pathologists
face several challenges in providing assessment and treatment
services to such people. Challenges include facilitating
equitable access to services and providing appropriate
management within a changing social and economic context.
Telerehabilitation has the potential to deliver services in the
home or local community via videoconferencing and through
interactive computer-based therapy activities. This form of
service delivery has the capacity to optimize functional
outcomes by facilitating generalization of treatment effects
within the person's everyday environment, and enable monitoring
of communication and swallowing behaviours on a long-term
basis. A number of image-based telerehabilitation applications
have been used in the management of adult neurogenic speech and
language disorders, stuttering, voice disorders, speech and
language disorders in children, laryngectomy and swallowing
dysfunction. Further development of such applications and other
computer-based therapies, cost-benefit and cost-effectiveness
analyses, and professional education are needed if
telerehabilitation is to become an integral part of
speech-language pathology practice.
Phenomenology of abnormal
movements in stuttering.
Riva-Posse P, Busto-Marolt L,
Schteinschnaider A, Martinez-Echenique L,
Cammarota A, Merello M.
Neuroscience Department, Movement
Disorders Section, Raúl Carrea Institute for Neurological
Research, FLENI, Montañeses 2325, 1428AQK, Ciudad de Buenos
Aires, Argentina.
BACKGROUND: Stuttering is often accompanied
by involuntary movements, abnormal gestures or changes in
facial expression. OBJECTIVE: To describe the incidence and
phenomenology of abnormal movements (AMs) in stuttering
patients. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Eighty-five consecutive
patients with stuttering and 119 normal controls videotaped and
subsequently reviewed, in which AMs were classified as
voluntary or involuntary, and as concurrent or unrelated to
speech. Movement phenomenology was correlated with disease
severity. RESULTS: Of 85 stuttering patients studied, 51.7% had
AMs and 22 more than one AM. Sixty-six different AMs were
identified, of which 83.3% occurred during speech, 72.7% were
classified as involuntary, and 27.2% as voluntary. Of 38
involuntary movements concurrent to speech, 25 were originally
perceived as voluntary, but had since become involuntary
through repeated use during stuttering. All involuntary
movements not concurrent to speech fulfilled criteria for tics.
CONCLUSION: AMs occurring during stuttering were not always
involuntary; movements not concurrent with speech clearly
fulfilled clinical criteria for tics and were similar in
incidence to normal controls. Inverse correlation was found
between conscious control of movement during speech and
stuttering severity. Many involuntary movements occurring
during speech were clearly referred by patients as initially
voluntary early on in the development of their speech disorder
(starters or unblockers), underlining the importance of
repetitive use of complex motor sequences as a source for
putative involuntary movement genesis.
Speech and non-speech activities in
stuttering: a preliminary study]
Andrade CR, Sassi FC, Juste FS,
Meira MI.
Departamento de Fisioterapia,
Fonoaudiologia e Terapia Ocupacional, Faculdade de Medicina,
Universidade de São Paulo, SP, Brazil. clauan@usp.br
BACKGROUND: stuttering. AIM: to compare
muscle activation in fluent and stuttering individuals during
speech and non-speech tasks. METHOD: six adults divided in two
groups: G1 - three fluent individuals; G2 - three stuttering
individuals. Muscle activity (surface electromyography) was
captured by disposable electrodes fixed in four regions.
Testing situations: muscle rest tension, speech reaction time,
non-verbal activity, verbal activity. RESULTS: There was no
significant statistical difference between the groups for the
rest tension; G2 present longer speech reaction times; G2
presented muscle activity during the non-verbal task similar to
that observed during rest; Muscle activity of G1 and G2 during
the verbal task demonstrated to be similar. CONCLUSION: these
results suggests that for G2 there is a poor control of timing
for the coordination of motor processes.
|